新概念雙語:看扒手如何欺騙你的大腦
來源: 環(huán)球網(wǎng)校 2019-12-03 09:55:13 頻道: 新概念

Pickpockets use much more than sleight of hand, says Caroline Williams, they hack your brain’s weaknesses。

My mother has eyes in the back of her head. She also taught me from an early age to be suspicious of strange men, especially when they give you presents. Which makes it all the more surprising that a “nice man” bearing flowers managed to swipe 20 euros from her purse, while she was holding it in her hands and looking straight at it。

“He said he was collecting for a church charity so I pulled out a euro,” she explains. “He said “no, no, that’s too much” and offered to look in my purse to find a smaller coin. He must have slid out the 20 euro note at the same time. I didn’t even notice until an hour later. I felt so stupid。”

But she needn’t feel bad. According to neuroscientists our brains come pretty much hard-wired to be tricked, thanks to the vagaries of our attention and perception systems. In fact, the key requirement for a successful pickpocket isn’t having nifty fingers, it’s having a working knowledge of the loopholes in our brains. Some are so good at it that researchers are working with them to get an insight into the way our minds work。

The most important of these loopholes is the fact that our brains are not set up to multi-task. Most of the time that is a good thing – it allows us to filter out all but the most important features of the world around us. But neuroscientist Susana Martinez-Conde, the author of the book Sleights of Mind, says that a good trickster can use it against you. She should know: as a researcher at the Laboratory of Visual Neuroscience in Arizona, she has studied how Las Vegas stage pickpocket Apollo Robbins performs his tricks。

“When Apollo gets someone on stage,” she says, “he is making them look at things, he is talking to them, he is touching their body, he is coming very close to them and producing an emotional response as he is entering their personal space… It’s complete attentional overload。”

So while sleight of hand helps, it’s as much about capturing all of somebody’s attention with other movements. Street pickpockets also use this effect to their advantage by manufacturing a situation that can’t help but overload your attention system. A classic trick is the ‘stall’, used by pickpocketing gangs all over the world. First, a ‘blocker’, walks in front of the victim (or ‘mark’) and suddenly stops so that the mark bumps into them. Another gang member will be close behind and will bump into both of them and then start a staged argument with the blocker. Amid the confusion one or both of them steal what they can and pass it to a third member of the gang, who quickly makes off with the loot。

據(jù)英國BBC網(wǎng)站報(bào)道,卡羅琳·威廉姆斯(Caroline Williams)稱扒手不僅手法熟練,還熟知你大腦思維的弱點(diǎn)。

我母親能感知背后發(fā)生的事情,仿佛她后腦也長了眼睛似的。從小她就教我要疑心陌生人,尤其是那些送你禮物的陌生人。所以,讓我尤其吃驚的是,一個(gè)手持鮮花的“好人”能從我媽錢包里順走20塊錢,當(dāng)時(shí)我媽正忙著欣賞握在自己手里的鮮花。

“他說他在為教堂慈善活動(dòng)籌款,所以我掏了1歐元出來,”我母親解釋,“他說‘不不,太多了’,然后說要看看我的錢包,只要個(gè)小硬幣就夠了。他肯定在這時(shí)將20歐的紙幣偷走。一個(gè)小時(shí)后我才發(fā)現(xiàn),我太笨了。”

但她不需要感到很糟糕。神經(jīng)系統(tǒng)科學(xué)家稱因?yàn)槲覀兊淖⒁饬透兄到y(tǒng)出現(xiàn)異常行為,而我們的大腦認(rèn)知極為模式化,所以容易被騙。實(shí)際上,一個(gè)扒手要成功偷竊的關(guān)鍵不在于靈活的手指,而是對我們大腦漏洞的認(rèn)知。有些扒手很擅長發(fā)現(xiàn)人的認(rèn)知弱點(diǎn),有些研究人員甚至與扒手合作,深入探解我們的大腦是如何工作的。

最重要的一個(gè)弱點(diǎn)是我們的大腦難以同時(shí)兼顧多項(xiàng)任務(wù)。通常這是好事——這樣我們就能篩選出身邊最重要的信息。神經(jīng)系統(tǒng)學(xué)家蘇珊娜·馬丁內(nèi)斯-康德(Susana Martinez-Conde)是《大腦詭計(jì)》(Sleights of Mind)的作者,她認(rèn)為這個(gè)大腦弱點(diǎn)讓我們?nèi)菀资茯_。她應(yīng)該知道:作為美國亞利桑那州(Arizona)視覺神經(jīng)學(xué)實(shí)驗(yàn)室(Laboratory of Visual Neuroscience)的研究人員,她已經(jīng)研究過美國拉斯維加斯(Las Vegas)表演扒手阿波羅·羅賓斯(Apollo Robbins)的表演伎倆。

馬丁內(nèi)斯-康德說:“阿波羅將觀眾引到臺(tái)上,讓他們看些東西,跟他們說話,接觸他們的身體,漸漸接近他們,營造某種情緒反應(yīng),讓阿波羅有一種進(jìn)入私人空間的感覺……就是增加一些信息而已。”

所以將一個(gè)人的全部注意力轉(zhuǎn)移到其他地方,加點(diǎn)手上技巧就成功了。街上的扒手也會(huì)營造某種只會(huì)增加你注意力負(fù)荷的場合,通過這種方法順利偷竊。全世界的扒手幫派都用的典型偷竊場景是“急停”。首先,一名“阻擋者”扒手走在受騙者(“目標(biāo)”)前面,然后突然停下來,與“目標(biāo)”撞個(gè)滿懷。另一扒手同伴緊跟其后也和“阻擋者”、“目標(biāo)”撞在一起,然后就開始扮演與“阻擋者”發(fā)生爭執(zhí),在混亂中他們其中一個(gè)或兩個(gè)偷到能摸得著的東西,然后轉(zhuǎn)手交給第三個(gè)同伴,第三個(gè)同伴再快速離開現(xiàn)場。

“People think it’s about distracting someone by making them look away but it’s actually about directing the mind towards something,” says James Brown, a stage pickpocket and hypnotist based in the UK. “If I wanted you to stop looking at something on the table it’s much easier for me to give you a good reason to look at something else. If I give you two or three things to focus on and the one I want you to avoid isn’t one of them, then that’s even better because now you have the illusion of choice。”

Other tactics are more psychological. Pickpockets tend to hang out near ‘beware of pickpockets’ signs, because the first thing people do when they read it is check they still have their valuables, helpfully giving away where they are. And in my mother’s case, the thief’s best trick was not coming across like a pickpocket. “He was a very nice guy and very personable. Not someone that would cause you to suspect,” she says。

Brown thinks confidence plays a major role too. “The biggest ploys used by theatrical pickpockets and the kind of street pickpockets that will actually engage with you, is simply an incredibly alluring display of confidence,” he says。

In theory, he adds, the power of suggestion alone is enough to persuade the most streetwise person to hand over their valuables. In 2009 a Russian bank employee gave over $80,000 of cash to a woman who apparently hypnotised her. “If you’ve got a bit of rapport with somebody and they trust you, it’s easy,” says Brown。

“人們會(huì)覺得讓他們看別處會(huì)使他們分心,實(shí)際上這會(huì)使他們的大腦分散注意力,”英國的扒手表演者和催眠師詹姆斯·布朗(James Brown)說,“如果我想讓你不再看桌上的東西,給你一個(gè)理由看別處會(huì)更容易。如果我讓你關(guān)注兩三樣?xùn)|西,但不包括我不希望你關(guān)注的東西,這更好,因?yàn)檫@會(huì)讓你產(chǎn)生有選擇的錯(cuò)覺。”

其他策略的心理作用更明顯。扒手喜歡在有“小心扒手”路標(biāo)的地方逛,因?yàn)槿藗兛吹铰窐?biāo)首先會(huì)檢查自己的貴重物品有沒有丟,于是就等于幫助了扒手,告訴他們目標(biāo)在哪兒。而我母親遇到的小偷最厲害的手段不是像一般的偶遇手段。我母親說:“他是一個(gè)友好的小伙子,很有風(fēng)度,不像那些會(huì)引人懷疑的人。”

布朗認(rèn)為自信對扒手也很重要,他說:“表演扒手和與你接觸的街上扒手最重要的策略是展示出富有魅力而又難以置信的自信。”

布朗補(bǔ)充,理論上光是暗示的力量就足以讓街上最聰明的行人交出財(cái)物。2009年,一個(gè)俄羅斯銀行職員被一個(gè)女人催眠,并將80,000美元的現(xiàn)金交給了這個(gè)女人。布朗說:“如果你對人比較和善而他們又信任你,那你就容易得手。”

Smart moves

On the stage, specific movements can also trick us. When Apollo Robbins started working with Martinez-Conde he told her that he had a hunch that certain ways he moved his hands seemed to affect how well he could direct a person’s attention。

If Robbins moved his hand through the air in a straight line between two points, he said, it was less effective at holding people’s attention on the end point than moving his hand in an arc motion. An arc motion would make people’s gaze stick to the curving hand and stay there, while a straight line would make their eyes flick back to the beginning and jump between the two。

Sure enough, eye tracking experiments showed that his hunch was right. But why? Martinez-Conde says that it is all down to the way different movements engage the visual system. Following an arc uses an eye movement called ‘smooth pursuit’, where the eye continuously follows an object. A straight line makes the eye move in a ‘saccade’, a fast movement where the eye moves from point A to point B in a fraction of a second。

“When we make a saccade our visual system is blind during the flight of the saccade, so you can see at the beginning and you can see at the end but while the eye is moving you cannot see,” she says. During smooth pursuit, however, there is no blind period, the eyes follow the moving object continuously from start to finish。

One explanation for why this makes us more likely to follow the hand, is that with a straight line, the eyes snap back to the beginning of the movement to try and fill in what the brain didn’t see during the movement. Whatever the explanation, it can be a very useful tool for a pickpocket. “Depending on what the pickpocket is interested in he may engage one or another type of motion, with or without engaging the person’s attention,” she says。

巧妙的動(dòng)作

在舞臺(tái)上,一些特殊的動(dòng)作也能欺騙我們。當(dāng)阿波羅·羅賓斯與馬丁內(nèi)斯-康德合作時(shí),他說他有預(yù)感,他移動(dòng)手的某些方式似乎會(huì)影響他控制某個(gè)人的注意力。

羅賓斯說,如果他在空氣中的兩點(diǎn)間直線移動(dòng)手,然后在某點(diǎn)停下,這動(dòng)作控制人們注意力的效果不如在空中做弧形移動(dòng);⌒我苿(dòng)能使人注視彎曲的手并在移動(dòng)終止點(diǎn)集中注意力,但直線移動(dòng)會(huì)讓人目光閃回起始點(diǎn),并在兩點(diǎn)間移動(dòng)。

的確,目光跟蹤實(shí)驗(yàn)證明羅賓斯的預(yù)感是對的。但為什么會(huì)這樣?馬丁內(nèi)斯-康德說這都因?yàn)椴煌膭?dòng)作會(huì)引起視覺系統(tǒng)的反應(yīng)。眼睛跟隨弧形移動(dòng)的滑動(dòng)叫“平滑移視”(smooth pursuit),此時(shí)眼睛持續(xù)盯著一個(gè)物體。直線移動(dòng)讓眼睛處在“眼掃視”(saccade)狀態(tài),眼球瞬間在A點(diǎn)和B點(diǎn)間快速移動(dòng)。

馬丁內(nèi)斯-康德說:“當(dāng)眼睛在快速掃視時(shí),我們的視覺系統(tǒng)是瞎的狀態(tài),所以你在移動(dòng)的起點(diǎn)和終點(diǎn)都能看得見,眼睛移動(dòng)時(shí)卻看不見。”然而在平滑移視中沒有看不見的時(shí)刻,隨著物體從運(yùn)動(dòng)開始至結(jié)束,眼球持續(xù)移動(dòng)。

對于為什么眼睛更容易跟著手動(dòng),一個(gè)解釋是手在直線移動(dòng)結(jié)束后,眼球會(huì)返回起始點(diǎn),嘗試補(bǔ)回剛才大腦看不見的移動(dòng)軌跡。不管是什么原因,這對扒手來說都是有用的手段。馬丁內(nèi)斯-康德說:“扒手會(huì)根據(jù)自己感興趣的東西而做一些其他動(dòng)作,以影響或不影響對方的注意力。”

Dirty tricks

Of course, if you want to play with someone’s powers of perception, a good time to try would be late at night when after a few drinks everything is already a little fuzzy. Brown says he spent a particularly fascinating night observing pickpockets outside nightclubs in London’s Trafalgar Square。

“They employ some clever tactics. A classic is that a girl comes up to you outside of a club and starts talking to you and as she’s doing it she starts rocking very gently. And the person thinks they are rocking so they compensate and start rocking and fall over. And she’s very kind and she helps you up and maybe her friend helps, too. You stumble off and the next morning you realise your watch has gone and your wallet is gone, everything’s gone。”

Having said all that, Brown is keen to point out that most thefts are opportunistic. “Having spent some time with the Romanian pickpocket gangs in London Bridge, it was fascinating to see how the level of skill is far less than you think. There’s a danger that these people are portrayed as being so skilled that it becomes almost endearing and elegant. Most of these people aren’t that at all, they are mostly opportunistically thieves。”

But, he warns, they are opportunistic enough to keep up with new technology. In the not too distant future, hacking contactless debit cards could prove just as fruitful for thieves as hacking our minds。

“Rather than take your wallet and get £50-60 and run the risk of being caught, why not walk through a busy place and just tap everybody’s pocket? If you took £19.99 off everybody, which is the limit of the contactless cards, that would be a very lucrative day right there. It’s all a little bit frightening。”

Still, knowing about all these tricks can make you a little less likely to have your valuables pilfered. At the very least, Brown says, it’s an idea not to zone out too much in public. “A street thief will avoid like the plague people who are demonstrating a very open awareness of their environment. The man on the tube who is looking around, being very aware, they won’t go anywhere near,” And as my mother would no doubt remind you, it’s also an idea to keep away from strangers with flowers。

卑鄙把戲

當(dāng)然,如果你想和別人玩弄一下感知力量,對方深夜喝了幾杯對一切感覺迷迷糊糊時(shí)是嘗試的好時(shí)機(jī)。布朗說他曾在一個(gè)特別美好的夜晚在倫敦特拉法加廣場(Trafalgar Square)的酒吧外觀察扒手。

布朗說:“他們會(huì)用些聰明的策略。有一個(gè)很典型的策略。酒吧外一個(gè)女孩向你走來,開始和你交談,并開始慢慢搖晃你。你會(huì)覺得自己在搖晃,想保持平衡,也開始搖晃起來,然后就跌到。那個(gè)女孩好心來扶你,或者她的朋友也會(huì)幫忙。最后你跌跌撞撞倒下,第二天早上發(fā)現(xiàn)手表不見了,錢包不見了,所有東西都不翼而飛。”

布朗說了這么多,是想指出多數(shù)偷竊都是投機(jī)的。他說:“我在倫敦塔橋觀察羅馬尼亞扒手幫很久了,他們的技巧其實(shí)沒有你想的那么厲害,我挺吃驚的。這些人被描述得那么厲害,甚至讓人覺得可愛優(yōu)雅,這挺危險(xiǎn)的。這些扒手大多數(shù)都不是這樣的,他們大多是投機(jī)取巧的小偷。”

但是布朗提醒我們這些扒手伺機(jī)而動(dòng),也會(huì)使用最新的科技。在不久的將來,偷取無接觸式借記卡對小偷來說與入侵我們大腦思維一樣有成效。

布朗稱:“偷走錢包,拿走五六十英鎊,還要冒著被捉的風(fēng)險(xiǎn),與之相比,為什么不走進(jìn)熙攘的地方拍拍每個(gè)人的口袋?如果從每個(gè)人身上拿走19.99鎊——這是無接觸式借記卡的上限,這樣一天在那兒就能掙不少。這些聽起來都讓人有點(diǎn)害怕。”

但了解一下所有這些偷竊手段能讓你被偷貴重物品的幾率降低一些。布朗說,至少要知道在公眾場合盡量不要分神,“街上的小偷會(huì)像躲瘟疫一樣躲開那些對四周保持警覺的人。地鐵里四處張望的人,扒手是不會(huì)靠近的。”我母親的案例無疑也會(huì)提醒你注意與拿著花的陌生人保持距離。

最近更新
熱點(diǎn)推薦